Introduction

Diabetes mellitus is a significant and growing problem worldwide affecting more than 25 million people in the USA, with an estimated lifetime prevalence of one-third for individuals born in 2000 [1]. A related condition, obesity, is also a growing problem in industrialised countries, affecting over one-third of the population, with an additional third being overweight [2]. There is a clear need for understanding the underlying mechanisms of and identifying effective therapies to treat type 2 diabetes and obesity.

Recently, incretin therapies have come to the forefront of potential treatments for type 2 diabetes and obesity [3]. The major source of endogenous glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) in humans is the intestinal L cell [3]. Accumulating evidence from both animal and human studies has shown that GLP-1 has an anorectic effect and may lead to weight reduction [4]. The GLP-1 analogue liraglutide has been approved by the US Food and Drug Administration, at a dose of 1.8 mg, for use in individuals with diabetes as an adjunct to diet and exercise [5]. Most recently, liraglutide 3.0 mg has been approved for the treatment of obesity.

In addition to acting on pancreatic and gastric targets, GLP-1 and liraglutide may act on the brain to produce satiety and suppress food intake [6, 7]. In rats and monkeys, GLP-1 receptors have been identified in several areas of the brain, including the hypothalamic nuclei and brainstem [8]. In mice, the weight-reducing effects of liraglutide were confirmed to be induced centrally, as this effect was moderated by the presence of neuronal GLP-1 receptors [9]. GLP-1 receptors have been identified in the hypothalamus, striatum, midbrain and hindbrain of primates but expression of these receptors has not been studied in humans [10]. The central effects of liraglutide also remain to be confirmed in humans, in whom the control of feeding involves other brain areas such as the prefrontal cortex which is responsible for cognitive control and decision-making, a rather complicated system which cannot be studied in mice or rats. The reward system also differs between mice and humans. Thus, although GLP-1 has been found to act on the mesolimbic dopamine system of mice [11], this remains to be studied in humans.

In this study, we used immunohistochemistry to explore whether GLP-1 receptors exist in human brains. We also investigated whether liraglutide administration could change the functional activation (using functional MRI [fMRI]) of brain centres important in energy homeostasis. A low dose of liraglutide was administered in a 2.5-week-long, dose-escalation, randomised, placebo-controlled, crossover study in diabetic individuals. To optimise this study, we gave the highest dosage (1.8 mg [as 3.0 mg had not yet been approved]) for a short time period (3 days) so that although the study took place at steady state and after any nausea initially caused by the medication, no weight loss would have occurred at the end of the study period and changes in body weight, per se, would not have confounded the results.

Methods

Study 1

Samples of hypothalamus, medulla oblongata and parietal cortex were obtained from discarded tissue taken from 22 autopsies (13 men, 9 women, aged 34–95 years) carried out less than 24 h after death. The selected paraffin blocks were analysed for the presence of GLP-1 receptors using standard immunohistochemical methods, employing the antibody against the human GLP-1 receptor (MAb 3F52) as previously described [1012]. See electronic supplemental material (ESM) Methods for further details. To further validate the results, fixed sections were blocked and another GLP-1 receptor antibody (MAb28141; R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) was used to confirm staining.

Study 2

Nine men and nine women with type 2 diabetes (defined as fasting plasma glucose > 6.94 mmol/l and/or HbA1c > 6.5% or 47.5 mmol/mol) provided written informed consent to participate in this randomised, crossover, placebo-controlled, double-blind study, approved by the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC) Institutional Review Board (details in ESM Fig. 1). Participants first came in for a screening visit to the BIDMC clinical research centre (CRC) to ensure that they met the inclusion/exclusion criteria for the study. See ESM Methods for further details.

After the screening visit, participants were randomised to receive either liraglutide or placebo for the first arm of the crossover study. They received the opposite treatment in the second arm of the study. Thus, all 18 participants completed both the liraglutide and placebo visits. Both arms were identical and separated by a wash-out period (minimum 3 weeks, maximum 3 months). Each arm consisted of four visits in total. At the first three visits, separated from each other by 1 week, physical examination and blood draws were performed for monitoring safety. To minimise side effects (e.g. nausea), the dose of liraglutide was gradually increased (0.6 mg at the first visit, 1.2 mg at the second and 1.8 mg at the third). The final visit occurred 3 days after the third visit (i.e. when each participant had been on 1.8 mg liraglutide for 3 days). At the final visit of each arm, participants attended the BIDMC CRC for a visit during which an overnight fast of at least 12 h was followed by an fMRI scan during the fasting state. Before and after the scan, participants used a visual analogue scale (VAS) to record subjective feelings of hunger, appetite and nausea. See ESM Methods for further details. Detailed anthropometric data (e.g. dual energy x-ray absorptiometry) were collected and resting metabolic rate was recorded.

Biochemical measurements and analysis

Fasting blood was drawn by venipuncture by a registered nurse. Samples were immediately processed for plasma and serum isolation according to standard operating procedures and stored at −80°C until analysis as previously described (ESM Table 1; [13, 14]). All samples and standards were assayed in duplicate and only results with a CV < 15% were used.

Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS; IBM Corporation; Armonk, NY, USA) v.19 and first summarised with descriptive statistics. The treatment effect was assessed using a general linear mixed model. The variables of treatment, visit and sequence were included in the model as fixed effects and patient-within-sequence was included as a random effect. Baseline values from the first visits in each arm were included as covariates. The values of anthropometric, clinical and laboratory variables at visit 4 and visit 8 were compared and used as dependent variables. Dependent variables that did not fulfil the normality assumptions were log-transformed before analysis. VAS and neurocognitive test results, collected at the endpoints for each phase (visits 4 and 8), were analysed with paired t tests between the treatment and placebo conditions. See ESM Methods for further details.

fMRI protocol and analysis

MRI scanning took place at the Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston University School of Medicine, using a 3-T Philips Intera whole-body MRI (Philips Medical Systems, Best, the Netherlands) equipped with a 32-channel Synergy-L Sensitivity Encoding (SENSE) head coil. See ESM Methods for further details.

The fMRI protocol consisted of six runs during which participants viewed blocks of images of highly desirable foods high in energy or fat content (e.g. cakes, onion rings and other similar foods), less desirable foods low in energy or fat content (e.g. vegetables and fruits) or non-food items (e.g. flowers, rocks and trees). Participants reported their response to each image (how much they liked each image, on a scale of 1–3), using a response box held in their right hand. Blocks consisted of five images, each image being shown for 3 s (15 s total for each block), and 18 blocks were shown during each of the six runs.

Blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) data was pre-processed using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping; The Wellcome Trust Centre of Neuroimaging, London, UK). The contrast images (highly desirable > less desirable food images and all food [both highly desirable and less desirable] cues > non-food images) of the first-level analysis were used for the second-level group statistics. At the second level, we performed paired t tests between participants’ scans while on liraglutide and placebo for the two contrasts (highly desirable food cues > less desirable food cues and all food cues > non-food cues). See ESM Methods for further details.

Neurocognitive testing

Neurocognitive testing was done after the completion of the fasting MRI scan and the consumption of a meal on the CANTAB device (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) (ESM Table 2).

Results

Study 1: expression of GLP-1 receptors in the human brain

In the parietal cortex, GLP-1 receptors stained with various intensity all neurons (small pyramidal, large pyramidal and polymorphous cells) in all cases examined (Table 1, Figs 1 and 2). Large pyramidal cells were usually the most immunoreactive while the polymorphous cells stained weakly and in two cases faintly. Glial cells (oligodendrocytes and astrocytes) were not immunoreactive in any case.

Table 1 Results of immunohistochemical analyses for areas of the hypothalamus, medulla oblongata and parietal cortex
Fig. 1
figure 1

Expression of GLP-1 receptors in human brain tissue. AP, area postrema; ARC, arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus; DMNV, dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus; LPC, large pyramidal cells of the parietal cortex; NSol, nucleus solitarius; PmC, polymorphous cells of the parietal cortex; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus; SPC, small pyramidal cells of the parietal cortex; VMH, ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus. White bars with grey border, no staining; light grey bars, weak staining; dark grey bars, mild staining; black bars, strong staining; diagonal stripe bars, very strong staining

Fig. 2
figure 2

Staining for the GLP-1 receptor (magnification × 400) in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (a), ventromedial hypothalamus (b), parietal cortex in small (c) and large pyramidal cells (d); staining in islets of Langerhans as a positive control (e) and glial and pyramidal cells without MAb 3F52 as a negative control (f). The arrow in (e) points to the non-neuroendocrine cells outside islets of Langerhans, which do not display immunopositivity, thus making them a valid positive control

In the hypothalamus, the arcuate nucleus, paraventricular nucleus and the complex of ventromedial nucleus neurons were immunopositive in the vast majority of the cases (21/21, 17/18 and 18/18, respectively). Stain intensity varied from weak to very strong, with the paraventricular and arcuate nuclei displaying strong immunoreactivity in eight cases each. Even though the neurons of the ventromedial nuclei were less immunoreactive, they were the structures that presented a distinct paranuclear (Golgi-like) stain pattern.

Area postrema and the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus in the medulla oblongata expressed GLP-1 in all cases (10/10 and 11/11, respectively) with the former displaying vigorous stain intensity. The dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus moderately expressed the GLP-1 receptor antigen in the majority of the cases. Weak, almost faint, immunoreactivity for this antigen was also demonstrated in the nucleus solitarius (7/10 positive, of which five were weak/faint).

Study 2: metabolic impact of liraglutide

Eighteen adults (9 women; age 48.9 ± 2.4 years) with type 2 diabetes participated in this study. Weight, BMI and waist circumference remained unchanged with liraglutide treatment (Table 2). Immunoreactive GLP-1 was increased in response to liraglutide (F statistic [degrees of freedom]) (F[32]) = 40.8; p < 0.001). The energy intake of participants was less while on 1.8 mg liraglutide than while on placebo (placebo [averaged]: 7,456 ± 803 kJ/day [1,782 ± 192 kcal/day]; 0.6 mg: 7,209 ± 937 kJ/day [1,723 ± 224 kcal/day]; 1.2 mg: 6,862 ± 586 kJ/day [1,640 ± 140 kcal/day]; 1.8 mg: 6,000 ± 795 kJ/day [1,434 ± 190 kcal/day]; t statistic [degrees of freedom] t[7] = 2.11; p < 0.07, two-tailed; p < 0.03, one-tailed). Fasting blood glucose levels improved with liraglutide treatment (F[19] = 24.5; p < 0.001), although neither HbA1c nor fructosamine improved within this short time period. The decrease in glucose remained significant in the treatment group when adjusted for BMI (F[19] = 22.7; p < 0.001).

Table 2 Body composition data and hormonal measurements for each phase before (visit 1) and after 17 days of liraglutide or placebo (visit 4)

Side effects of liraglutide

Nausea was reported equally by participants while receiving liraglutide and placebo at earlier visits (n = 7 participants in each arm; 38.9%; χ 2 = 0; p > 0.999). VAS ratings of nausea were no different at the time of the MRIs (t[17] = 0.9; p < 0.27; liraglutide: 5.45 ± 0.61; placebo: 6.51 ± 0.73). Although amylase showed no changes, lipase increased with liraglutide (F[15] = 12.1; p < 0.003) within the normal range (Table 2).

Neurocognitive impact of liraglutide

There were no significant changes in any of the neurocognitive testing results when comparing liraglutide with placebo (Table 3). With a paired-samples t test, participants showed less inferior parietal cortex activation (297 mm3; XYZ: −48, −26, 46; z = 4.13) in response to highly desirable vs less desirable food images while on 1.8 mg liraglutide vs placebo at p < 0.001, uncorrected (Fig. 3). In a secondary analysis, using small volume corrections on data from highly desirable vs less desirable images in the fasting state, we saw similar decreases in activation to those obtained with exenatide reported by van Bloemendaal et al [15] for insula and putamen (Fig. 4). Notably, we did not observe any changes in the hypothalamus.

Table 3 Outcomes of neurocognitive testing while on liraglutide vs placebo
Fig. 3
figure 3

Brain activation in inferior parietal cortex in response to highly desirable compared with less desirable food images decreases with liraglutide treatment (whole-brain paired-samples t test). BOLD contrasts are superimposed on a T1 structural image in neurological orientation. The colour bar represents voxel T value

Fig. 4
figure 4

In a secondary analysis, small volume corrections were performed on contrast images for highly vs less desirable food cues in the insula and putamen; to be compared with results from van Bloemendaal et al (Fig. 3) [15]. The y-axis represents effect size of the activation (z scores). BOLD contrasts are superimposed on a T1 structural image in neurological orientation. The colour bar represents voxel T value. Lira, liraglutide

Impact of neuroimaging

Using the VAS for rating hunger, participants rated themselves as being fuller when taking liraglutide while fasting (liraglutide: 3.97 ± 0.77; placebo: 2.12 ± 0.67; t[17] = 3.25, p < 0.006). The participants trended towards feeling that it would be less pleasant to eat during the fasting state on liraglutide, although this rating did not reach significance (liraglutide: 5.45 ± 0.61; placebo: 6.52 ± 0.06; t[17] = 1.94, p < 0.09, one-tailed based on an a priori hypothesis for a decrease). However, while receiving liraglutide, participants’ ratings of how pleasant it would be to eat correlated with the parietal activation shown in the whole-brain analysis (i.e. those who showed the least activation of the parietal cortex in response to highly desirable food images thought it would be least pleasant to eat) (r = 0.820; p < 0.024). In whole-brain regression analyses of the VAS ratings and brain activity during the fasting-state scan while on liraglutide, when comparing response to images of highly desirable vs less desirable food we observed that participants’ ratings for concepts related to hunger and appetite showed a positive correlation with activation in many brain areas, including the parietal cortex, precuneus, cuneus and other areas of the occipital cortex (Table 4; Fig. 5a–c). On the other hand, participants’ ratings of nausea inversely correlated with brain activation in the cingulate cortex, cuneus, precuneus and some areas of prefrontal cortex (Table 4; Fig. 5d).

Table 4 Results from a whole-brain regression analysis and VAS data during the liraglutide fMRI session for the contrast of highly desirable vs less desirable food images in the fasting state
Fig. 5
figure 5

Results from a whole-brain regression with VAS data during the liraglutide fMRI session for the contrast of highly desirable vs less desirable food images in the fasting state. Participants’ ratings of the VAS questions along a 10 cm line were regressed with whole-brain activations: ‘How hungry do you feel right now?’ (a); ‘How pleasant would it be to eat right now?’ (b); ‘How much do you think you could eat right now?’ (c) and ‘How nauseous do you feel right now?’ (d). Hot colours show positive correlations, cold colours show negative correlations. BOLD contrasts are superimposed on a T1 structural image in axial sections from z = −15 to z = 60, in neurological orientation. The colour bars represent voxel T value

Discussion

This study provides novel information on the expression of GLP-1 receptors in human brains as well as on how liraglutide 1.8 mg may influence short-term appetite regulation among individuals with diabetes. This is the first randomised, placebo-controlled, crossover trial to examine potential neural mechanisms activated by subcutaneous liraglutide therapy. More specifically, we observed changes in activation of the parietal cortex, insula and putamen in response to highly palatable food cues with liraglutide as compared with placebo. Our results suggest that liraglutide may exert central effects by decreasing attention to highly palatable foods, given decreased parietal cortex activation; additional central mechanisms will need to be explored more fully by means of larger trials and/or using higher doses of liraglutide.

Expression of GLP-1 receptors in the human brain

We observed expression of GLP-1 receptors in the hypothalamic nuclei and medulla oblongata of humans, confirming the findings of studies in rodents and primates [1012]. This suggests that GLP-1 analogues may confer some of their effects on appetite through actions on the hypothalamus. The identification of GLP-1 receptors in the area postrema may also explain why nausea is a relatively common side effect of GLP-1 analogues [16], considering the area postrema has a large role in nausea and vomiting [17].

Interestingly, we observed for the first time GLP-1 receptors on neurons in the parietal cortex, suggesting our findings of altered parietal activation with liraglutide could be a direct rather than an indirect effect (e.g. acting through hypothalamic circuitry) of the medication. This needs further study. Our findings are strengthened by the translational finding that activation of this area in fMRI was directly correlated with the desirability of food to patients. Our findings are also distinct from those of other studies, using the same antibody, reporting that there were no GLP-1 receptors in the parietal cortex of non-human primates [10]. Although we did not see changes in the hypothalamus in response to food cues while participants were taking liraglutide, this may be due to the limitations of MRI, due partly to the size of this structure and partly to its proximity to the sinuses, which creates artefacts [18]. As far as identification of GLP-1 in the human brain, Drucker and Asa described in 1988 GLP-1 (also known as GCG) mRNA-positive neurons in the region of dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve in the medulla oblongata [19]. Recently, GLP-1-immunopositive neurons within the human medulla oblongata have been observed in two patients [20]. Immunoreactivity was displayed in the nucleus of the solitary tract, in area postrema and in the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve. We will expand our findings to determine whether the GLP-1 receptor also exists in these areas and explore its presence in more patients.

Metabolic impact of liraglutide

In our trial GLP-1 immunoreactivity in the circulation of individuals increased in a step-wise manner with increasing doses of liraglutide. Additionally, fasting blood sugar levels improved without weight loss in this short course of 1.8 mg liraglutide therapy. No significant changes were noted in fructosamine, HbA1c, lipid profiles, blood pressure or anthropometric measurements, such as BMI and fat mass, as expected with the short duration of therapy designed to eliminate weight loss as a potential confounder. We did notice decreased energy intake with the 1.8 mg liraglutide dose with no accompanying changes in resting energy expenditure; this would likely lead to weight loss with a study of longer duration.

Central mechanisms of liraglutide

We observed that, in response to liraglutide treatment, parietal activation decreased when participants viewed images of highly desirable food. The inferior parietal cortex is a well-known part of the fronto-parietal attention network, which demonstrates increased activation to salient or important and, in this case, highly desirable stimuli [2123]. Thus, we suggest that this decreased activation represents the decreased significance or appeal of more desirable (high-energy) foods while participants were taking liraglutide. Parietal activation in response to images of high-energy foods inversely correlated with weight loss in another fMRI study, suggesting that decreased attention to these images leads to greater weight loss [24]. Additionally, our participants rated themselves as feeling fuller in the fasting state while receiving liraglutide and had a lower energy intake with increasing liraglutide doses, altogether suggesting that they would indeed lose weight. Those with the least inferior parietal activation identified in the whole-brain analysis also reported it would be less pleasant to eat while on liraglutide during the fasting state, further supporting this notion. This change in activation may indicate neural changes that could proceed to and predict future weight loss with liraglutide, a notion to be fully evaluated by long-term studies.

Further supporting these findings are our whole-brain regression results, which show that activation of areas related to attention and saliency processing, including the occipital cortex, cuneus, precuneus and parietal cortex, were correlated with ratings of hunger and appetite. Greater activation in these areas in response to highly desirable vs less desirable food cues during the fasting state while on liraglutide were associated with higher levels of hunger, larger appetite and a feeling of how pleasant it would be to eat. These occipital areas (cuneus, precuneus and occipital cortex) are known to respond to food cues and this has been attributed to the emotional salience of food cues [25, 26]. Indeed, increased activation of occipital cortex has also been linked with emotional images [27, 28]. This may indicate individual differences in response to liraglutide, where participants who have decreased activation in these areas show less hunger and appetite. Interestingly, ratings of nausea while on liraglutide correlated inversely with activations in some of the same attention-related brain areas, likely indicating that participants are less interested in food items while feeling nauseous, as would be expected. We also report changes in activation in sensorimotor areas, including the cingulate, superior frontal and somatosensory cortex, which are often linked with feelings of nausea [29, 30]. These may be deactivated in response to highly desirable food cues simply due to the feeling of nausea and lack of desirability of these types of foods when one is feeling nauseous. Additionally, nausea is a side effect of medication, so these changes might be a result of increased medication effects for certain individuals at 17 days.

There are limited other studies examining the effects of other GLP-1 analogues in the human brain. One study has demonstrated changes in resting-state hypothalamic connectivity with a single continuous infusion of exenatide in obese responders (e.g. those who consumed less food at a subsequent ad libitum buffet) [31]. A recent study using exenatide found changes in brain areas of the reward system, including the insula and orbital frontal cortex, with a similar task [15]. Nausea could be considered a confounder in that study which used a single dose, whereas in our study, nausea had subsided for most patients but weight loss had not yet occurred [15]. We did observe similar activation in the insula and putamen with the same small volume correction on our data. It remains to be seen whether statistically significant results would also be observed with a higher dose of liraglutide (3 mg) or more participants. More recently, another study comparing liraglutide with insulin showed similar decreases in activation to food cues after 10 days, but not 12 weeks, in the insula and putamen [32]. It is well known that the insula is involved in saliency processing and satiety [3335]. Insular activity has been shown to be modulated with ratings of hunger and fasted/fed states [34, 35], indicating a role in the salience of food images similar to the parietal cortex, consistent with its activity during non-food salient stimuli [36]. Some studies have suggested that the putamen may be involved in the reward processing of food [15, 3739] and have shown links between activation in the putamen and future weight gain [38]. Thus, since these areas decrease in activation on liraglutide, we would assume that participants find highly palatable foods less rewarding or relevant while on liraglutide. We report such data as only preliminary, however, and they need to be confirmed by future studies.

Limitations and future directions

We demonstrate a central role for GLP-1 analogues in mediating weight loss in humans. Results from immunohistochemical studies support the findings of our fMRI study, suggesting that liraglutide likely acts through the parietal cortex directly to decrease attention to highly palatable food cues. Due to the crossover design of our study with randomly assigned sequence, we are confident that potential covariates are well-controlled and data are appropriately analysed, since each individual serves as his or her own control. However, the strength of our findings could be amplified by the addition of more participants and/or with studies of longer duration.

We did not see any changes in fMRI activation of the hypothalamus with liraglutide, in contrast to immunohistochemistry findings in rodents and primates [10, 12]. This may be due to differences in terms of control of eating between rodents and humans; in the latter the homeostatic control of eating is often overridden by more complex cortical systems regulating the rewarding value of food [18]. This may also be a limitation of fMRI in general, as the hypothalamus is difficult to detect with typical fMRI measures due in part to its small size and in part to its proximity to the sinuses, which create artefacts [18].

The fMRI outcomes of our study need to be studied in different time frames after administration to explore possible sequential effects (e.g. changes at first dose and after longer-term use). Additionally, comparative studies to explore potential differences between types of GLP-1 analogues would be useful. Also, different doses (e.g. 3.0 mg vs 1.8 mg liraglutide) need to be examined further. It is possible that more pronounced changes could have been observed with the newly recommended higher dose of liraglutide (3.0 mg), which has been shown to cause weight loss more dramatically [40]. In fact, despite no difference in gastric emptying or nausea, the 3.0 mg dose of liraglutide caused greater weight loss than the 1.8 mg dose, suggesting greater central/brain changes [40]. Thus, future studies need to examine how this higher dose alters brain activations.

We did not see any significant changes with respect to neurocognition; the variable closest to significance was the stop signal reaction time (i.e. the most appropriate test as per our a priori hypotheses), which can be used for future power calculations with higher doses and/or longer duration. The stop signal reaction time is a measure of inhibitory control, where a lower time indicates greater inhibitory control, which has been shown to be impaired with obesity [41]. This may indicate that with higher doses and/or more participants, liraglutide may increase inhibitory control, but this will need to be confirmed.